Southern Illinois University David G. King
Annotated list of publications and presentations

Most peer-reviewed publications listed below are available somewhere on the internet, at least in abstract.  Meeting presentations (posters and slide shows; not peer-reviewed) are by nature ephemeral, except for the pdfs archived here.  Full-text PDFs may be requested by email. 

Each of the listings below is followed by a brief personal comment.  Reading from the bottom up, these comments sketch out the trajectory of my research career.

before 1981    |   1981-1990    |   1991-2000    |   2001-2010    |   since 2010    |  Epilogue

The topics of these publications might appear diverse and somewhat esoteric, but they are all united my desire to understand patterns of structure and genetics that enable evolutionary adaptation, especially the marvellous evolutionary malleability of animal behavior.  And they are all inspired by an appreciation for "extremely minute parts so shaped and situated as to form a marvelous organ" (to quote Marcello Malpighi, a seventeenth-century microscopist). 

(2024)  King, D.G. Mutation protocols share with sexual reproduction the physiological role of producing genetic variation within 'constraints that deconstrain'The Journal of Physiology, https://doi.org/10.1113/JP285478.  [abstract]  [free-access pdf -- at least temporarily]

Unexpectedly, after nine years of retirement I stumbled into an invitation to submit an essay for a special "physiology of evolution" issue of The Journal of Physiology.  The essay was published online on 4 January, 2024.  Written for J. Physiology readership, it reviews material explored in several previous publications (listed below, including abstracts from 1985 and 2010), with addition of some up-to-date references.

Some readers might find it incredible that the outdated evolutionary views discussed here still need to be addressed.  Yet even in 2023, in Nature Reviews Genetics 24:3, we can read that "Sturtevant's [1937] views on the evolution of mutation rates are likely to continue to influence future efforts to understand what remains a fundamental question in biology."  My primary impetus for this ms. was to address just such persistent influence.

(2015)  King, D.G.Might constrained mutability be as advantageous as sex?  (poster pdf).

This poster, like the two immediately below, takes direct aim at the classic but misleadingly simplistic assertion, famously declared by George Williams in 1966, that "[N]atural selection of mutation rates has only one possible direction, that of reducing the frequency of mutation to zero... So evolution takes place, not so much because of natural selection, but to a large degree in spite of it."

For several years, I thought that this post-retirement meeting presentation would represent my final effort to advocate for the idea of mutation protocols as metaptation.  This was somewhat disappointing, since posters are ephemeral (except as archived here).  However, in 2023 I was invited to contribute an article to a special issue on "physiology of evolution" in The Journal of Physiology

(In this pdf, the poster display follows the "Further reading" list.)

(2014)  King, D.G.A lesson from sex:  Abundant variation can be worth its high price.  (poster pdf)

This poster, like the ones immediately above and below, challenges the misguided presumption that, in Sturtevant's oft-quoted words from 1937, "the vast majority of mutations are unfavorable... [M]utations are accidents, and accidents will happen."  But this presumption ignores the simple fact that sexual reproduction is, fundamentally, a vast source of potentially advantageous mutations:  Practically every instance of meiotic recombination creates DNA sequences that have never previously existed.  And nearly all of these are NOT unfavorable. 

(Textbooks commonly exclude the products of reciprocal recombination by definition from the category of mutation, simply because they are seldom deleterious.  One example of such explicit exclusion can be found in the glossary of Douglas Futuyma's highly regarded textbook Evolutionary Biology, 3rd ed., 1998.)

(In this pdf, the poster follows an abstract and reading list.)

(2013b)  King, D.G.Whence variation?  Rethinking mutation and evolvability.  (poster pdf)

This poster, together with the two immediately above, takes direct aim at the classic but misguided dogma, reiterated as recently as 2012 by Elmore et al. (Genes Genomes Genetics 2: 1643-1649), that "a well established and supported tenet of evolutionary theory is that, because most new mutations are deleterious, selection in all organisms will act to reduce mutation rate toward the physiology- or selection-imposed minimum."

(In this pdf, an abstract and a reading list follow the poster.)

(2013a)  King, D.G. What can giant axons tell us about genetics and evolution?  International Society for Neuroethology Newsletter, July/August 2013, pp. 5-7.

A pair of giant axons in a small fly
(note additional large-axon pairs).

As my retirement approached, I prepared this essay -- illustrating distinctive patterns of nerve fibers in several species of flies -- as a "swan song" to my interest in specialized neuronal form and function.

The idea of individually specialized nerve cells has fascinated me throughout my career.  I was introduced to this concept during my first "rotation" in graduate school at UCSD, where I was assigned to record electrical activity from one particular cell (R-15, "the parabolic burster") in the abdominal ganglion of Aplysia.  My doctoral dissertation (1976b and 1976c, below) focused on several individually-identifiable cells in the stomatogastric ganglion of the California spiny lobster.  My postdoctoral work was directed toward understanding unique giant axons in Drosophila.  During the early 1980s, I began examining a variety of different fly species in hopes of pinning down the phylogenetic origin of those curious giant axons.  (Although a simple story of giant fiber evolution continues to elude me, the specimens collected for that purpose served admirably for my 1991 report on the evolutionary origin of the dipteran cardia.) 

Hoping to stimulate more widespread interest in the evolution of individually-identifiable nerve cells, I reported further details of fly neurons at several annual meetings of the Society for Neuroscience.  When those presentations received very little attention, I put neurobiology aside in favor of my transition to studies of evolutionary genetics.  Yet even this transition was indirectly driven by my desire to understand how individually-identifiable nerve cells could evolve, particularly when the totality of information in a genome would seem inadequate (at least to me) to specify such precision circuitry.

This essay expresses my conviction that the goal of understanding nervous system organization might benefit by addressing evolutionary processes.  (For a related, extended discussion, see my 2001 online-only essay, "Evolution of neural circuits." Also cf., 1988b, 2007c.)
 

What the devil determines each particular variation?  What makes a tuft of feathers come on a cock's head, or moss on a moss-rose?
Charles Darwin (letter to T.H. Huxley, 1859)

(2012)  King, D.G.    Indirect Selection of Implicit Mutation Protocols.  Annals of the New York Academy of Science 1267:45-52.   doi:  10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06615.x  [author preprint] [email for PDF: dgking@siu.edu]

This essay is the culmination of my work on the implications of indirect selection for evolutionary processes.  Subsequent poster presentations, at meetings held in 2013, 2014, 2015, and also listed above, have offered some further elaboration.  And a somewhat more forceful presentation is due to appear in 2024.
 

(2011d) King, D.G.  Indirect selection of local mutation rates (abstract only).  DIMACS Conference on Effects of Genome Structure and Sequence on the Generation of Variation and Evolution, August 9 - 11, 2011. 

In 2011 I was invited to participate in a conference at the Rutgers University Center for Discrete Mathematics and Theoretical Computer Science.  My abstract, initially published in proceedings of this conference, was subsequently expanded into an essay (2012, above), published in a collection edited by Lynn H. Caporale with the same title as the conference, "Effects of Genome Structure and Sequence on the Generation of Variation and Evolution," in the Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Volume 1267 (2012).
 

(2011c)  King, D.G.  Genetic Variation Among Developing Brain Cells.  Science E-Letter (published 16 May 2011). 

This letter-to-the-editor (continuing our campaign to publicize the evolutionary role of repetitive DNA) was submitted in response to a news article by Gretchen Vogel ( Science 332:300-301, 2011), "Do Jumping Genes Spawn Diversity?" (This is an e-letter, not appearing in print; it appears farther down on the same webpage as Vogel's article.)

(2011b)  King, D.G.  Light on genetic dark matter (letter to the editor).  Science News 179(7):30 (26 March 2011). 

Another letter-to-the-editor in our campaign to publicize the evolutionary role of repetitive DNA, this time in a science magazine published for the educated general public.
 

(2011a)  King, D.G.    Evolution of simple sequence repeats as mutable sites.  In: Anthony Hannan, ed., Tandem Repeat Polymorphisms: Genetic Plasticity, Neural Diversity and Disease, Landes Bioscience and Springer Science+Business Media.  Adv Exp Med Biol. (2012) 769:10-25.  [preprint]   [email for PDF: dgking@siu.edu]

Unconstrained by any publisher-imposed word limit, in this article I was able to develop a more thorough consideration of simple sequence repeat evolution than I have accomplished anywhere else, before or since (cf. 2012). 

Here, in addition to reviewing both classical and more-nuanced discussions of natural selection for mutation-rate, I consider the significance of the phylogenetic distribution of SSRs.  This distribution reveals evidence which I find especially compelling, that the evolution of SSRs has indeed been driven by more than mutation pressure and genetic drift.  Positive (albeit indirect) selection -- favoring, preserving, and shaping the mutagenic potential of SSRs -- has surely been involved as well. 

(Due to citations added in proof, the author preprint available here differs from the print version of this article.)
 

Image by Milo Winter, Aesop's Fables, 1919
(adapted from Wikipedia Commons)

(2010b)  King, D.G.    Metaptation: Metaphors for genome evolution.  In: Structural and Functional Diversity of the Eukaryotic Genome, Verhandlungen des naturforschenden Vereines in Brünn, Specialausgabe, p. 40. 

This abstract, which offers an updated definition for the word "metaptation" (first introduced in 1985), was written for an international workshop held at the Mendel Museum in the former St. Thomas Abbey (Brno, Czech Republic) where Gregor Mendel grew his peas.  It might be my only publication to include an exclamation point! 

My invitation to speak at this workshop came from Ed Trifonov, who had been coauthor on our 2006a article in The Implicit Genome.  The image at right links to the slideshow for my presentation.  Most of the other presentations at this meeting were dense with molecular information that went way over my head.  Cheeky of me, a neuroscientist, to speak at a molecular genetics seminar in Mendel's abbey. 

I took advantage of my post-retirement 2024 Journal of Physiology paper to slip the word "metaptation" into mainstream biology literature.
 

(2010a)  King, D.G.  A hypertext resource for self-directed learning.  (poster pdf)

Throughout my career, my principal teaching assignment has been histology.  In 2000 I began building a website emulating my role as instructor, to help students learn that subject.  This poster describes the resulting resource.

The website itself, consisting of several hundred webpages of extensively hyperlinked text and images, was initially posted at a time when internet resources were not as extensive as they have since become.  By 2010 this website had acquired sufficient popularity that a Google search for the single word "histology" returned this site in first place out of several million hits.  This website remains a work in progress.  For obscure reasons it was deleted from the SIU School of Medicine webserver in 2021; nevertheless, after being resurrected at its current url, histology.siu.edu, it has regained its visibility to Google searching.  I have been extremely gratified to receive "fan mail" from around the world, expressing appreciation for the quality of this site.
 

(2009b)  King, D.G.Why do so many neurodevelopmental genes contain repeat-based mutable sites?  (poster pdf)

I prepared this poster for the 2009 meeting of the Society for Neuroscience (the last such meeting which I have attended), hoping to call some attention to especially intriguing results in two papers published that same year.
 

(2009a)  King, D.G., and Y. Kashi  Heretical DNA sequences?  Science 326: 229-230.  

This letter-to-the-editor was written to challenge simplistic application of the "conservation equals function" paradigm in evolutionary genetics, while publicizing the intriguing results recently published by Riley & Krieger. (cf., 2009 poster).
 

(2008b)  King, D.G.Facilitating Evolution: Adaptation and Simple Sequence Repeat Polymorphism.  (poster pdf)

This poster was prepared for an ecology symposium; an abstract and a reading list follow the poster.
 

(2008a)  Fondon III, J.W., E.A.D. Hammock, A.J. Hannan, and D.G. King  Simple sequence repeats: Genetic modulators of brain function and behavior.  Trends in Neurosciences 31: 328-334.  doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.03.006.  [authors' prepublication MSWord document] [email for PDF: dgking@siu.edu]

This review emphasizes the applicability of our tuning-knob hypothesis (1997b, 1997c, 2006a) in the domain of neuroscience.  My principal roles in the preparation of this article were recruiting coauthors with expertise on the effects of simple sequence repeat variation, editing the tens of thousands of words provided by these coauthors into the word-limit imposed by the journal, and writing the introduction and conclusion.
 

(2007c)  King, D.G.What's so special about giant fibers?  (slide show pdf)

This slide show was prepared for a symposium on myelin (cf. evolution of myelin), at the National Evolutionary Synthesis Center (the precursor to TriCEM).  Content is similar to 2013a (above).  My invitation to this symposium came from Dan Hartline, who had been a professor down the hall from Allen Selverston's laboratory at UC San Diego while I was a graduate student in that lab.
 

(2007b)  King, D.G., and Y. Kashi  Mutation rate variation in eukaryotes: evolutionary implications of site-specific mechanisms.  Nature Reviews Genetics 8 (November 2007) | doi:10.1038/nrg2158-c1

This is the second of two short essays published in 2007, in response to published papers which failed to address overwhelming evidence for extensive variation in localized, site-specific mutation rates.  Unfortunately, the significance of such sites is still being ignored years later (e.g., Zhang 2022, also in Nature Reviews Genetics).  Mutation rate continues to be treated as a single, unitary parameter, while Sturtevant's obsolete 1937 dictum -- that "mutations are accidents, and accidents will happen" -- persists as misleading dogma (cf., Indirect selection of implicit mutation protocols, above).
 

(2007a)  King, D.G., and Y. Kashi  Mutability and Evolvability:  Indirect selection for mutability.  Heredity 99:123-124. doi:10.1038/sj.hdy.6800998.  [email for PDF: dgking@siu.edu]

This is the first of two short essays published in 2007, in response to published papers which failed to address overwhelming evidence for extensive variation in localized, site-specific mutation rates.  Unfortunately, the significance of such sites is still being ignored years later (e.g., Zhang 2022).  Mutation rate continues to be treated as a single, unitary parameter, while Sturtevant's obsolete 1937 dictum -- that "mutations are accidents, and accidents will happen" -- persists as misleading dogma (cf., Indirect selection of implicit mutation protocols, above).
 

(2006e)  King, D.G.Facilitating Evolution: Simple Sequence Repeats Make Genes Adjustable.  (poster pdf).

While attending a conference at UC Berkeley to present this poster, I shared a hotel room with John Fondon who had made a splash in 2004 by reporting the role of repetitive DNA in morphological variation of dogs.  I later recruited Fondon as a coauthor for our Trends in Neuroscience paper (2008a).
 

(2006d)  Y. Kashi and D.G. King  Has simple sequence repeat mutability been selected to facilitate evolution?  Israel Journal of Ecology and Evolution 52:331-342.  [Abstract] [email for PDF: dgking@siu.edu]

This essay continued our campaign to publicize the evolutionary implications of simple sequence repeats.
 

(2006c)  Hammock, E.A.D., and D.G. King  Genes and neuroethology:  How can evolution adjust behavior?  International Society for Neuroethology Newsletter, November 2006, pp. 4-7.

This newsletter article highlighted new information relating simple sequence repeats to affiliative behavior in voles, as reported by Elizabeth Hammock in the previous year.  I later recruited Hammock as a coauthor for our Trends in Neuroscience paper (2008a).
 

(2006b)  Y. Kashi and D.G. King    Simple Sequence Repeats as Advantageous Mutators in EvolutionTrends in Genetics 22: 253-259.  [Abstract]  doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2006.03.005 [email for PDF: dgking@siu.edu]

This review updated our 1997 TiG review, with greater emphasis on evolutionary implications.  It has become our most highly-cited publication.

Evidence for such "advantageous mutators" has continued to accumulate.  Some of the more recent publications are cited in my 2024 article in The Journal of Physiology, which generalizes the concept to additional protocols for mutation besides tandem repeats.
 

(2006a)  King, D.G., E.N. Trifonov, and Y. Kashi  Tuning Knobs in the Genome:  Evolution of Simple Sequence Repeats by Indirect Selection.  In: Lynn H. Caporale, ed., The Implicit Genome, Oxford University Press.  [Abstract]   [Table of contents for The Implicit Genome]   [email for preprint: dgking@siu.edu]

Lynn Caporale offered the invitation to submit this essay, based on mutual interests established at her 1998 NYAS meeting, "Molecular strategies of biological evolution" (1999b).  In previous reviews coauthored with Kashi and Soller (listed below), I had depended for content almost entirely on those two coauthors.  Preparing to write this review finally gave me enough confidence to continue writing on my own.

Another essay in this volume, "Epilogue: An Engineering Perspective: The Implicit Protocols," by John Doyle, Marie Csete, and Lynn Caporale, introduced the "protocols" metaphor which became central to my 2012 essay in Annals NYAS.
 

(2002)  King, D.G., and J.G. Killian  Binocular rivalry, bipolar disorder, and aging:  Perceptual alternation rate correlates with age as well as with psychiatric diagnosis.  Program No. 803.4 Washington DC:  Society for Neuroscience.  [Abstract with graph of data]  [extended discussion]

Prompted by events in my personal life, I put aside my principal interest in evolutionary genetics for a three-year project summarized in this abstract.  The project was funded by the Brain & Behavior Research Foundation.  Its subject, binocular rivalry, is a curious phenomenon with implications for the neural substrate of consciousness (more).  Killian, a graduate student at the time, gave up a secure job to assist with this project.
 

(2001)  King, D.G.  Evolution of neural circuits.  SIUC Cognitive Science Colloquium.

This entry -- the text of a seminar presentation on October 5, 2001 -- summarizes most of the central ideas of my research career, both before and after this date.
 

(2000)  King, D.G.  Indirect selection on the mutational landscape: An evolved role for the mutability of repetitive DNA?  Poster #218, in Evolution 2000, (ed. M.J. Wade), Proceedings of the 2000 annual meeting of the Society for the Study of Evolution.  [extended abstract, pdf]

An extended abstract of this meeting presentation, including graphically illustrated results of a simple model of population genetics, was published in a CD-ROM of the proceedings of the 2000 meeting of the Society for the Study of Evolution.  (Unfortunately, the full contents of this CD do not appear to be available online.)  Disappointingly, this poster presentation garnered essentially no interest; reaching a broad audience with this idea has proven challenging (cf. 2007a and 2007b, above).
 

(1999c)  Krajewski, C., Fain, M., L. Buckley and King, D. G. (1999)  Dynamically heterogenous partitions and model-based phylogenetic inference:  An evaluation of analytical strategies with cytochrome b and ND6 gene sequences in cranes.  Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 13: 302-313.   [Abstract]

For this and a 1996 paper also coauthored with Krajewski, my only contribution was the coding of conceptually-simple computer programs which facilitated the comparison of DNA sequences.  Matt Fain and Larry Buckley were Krajewski's graduate students.
 

(1999b)  King, D.G.   Modelling selection for adjustable genes based on simple sequence repeats.  In: Molecular Strategies in Biological Evolution, Annals of the New York Academy of Science 870: 396-399.

This abstract was published in the proceedings of a 1998 meeting of the New York Academy of Sciences, "Molecular strategies in biological evolution," organized by Lynn Helena Caporale.  Prior to this meeting, I had written a computer program simulating a very simple model of population genetics.  Output from many runs of that program had convinced me that the speculative idea of "indirect selection" could plausibly occur as a real phenomenon.  A more elaborate version of this computer model was prepared for the 2000 meeting of the Society for the Study of Evolution. 

I had travelled to this meeting not knowing what to expect; at this time I still felt like a naive outsider in the field of molecular evolution.  But at this meeting I found that there existed a small community of biologists who shared my growing interest in molecular mechanisms that could plausibly function as strategies for evolution.  This discovery felt like a turning point in my career.  I was pleasantly astonished when the first speaker (James A. Shapiro) cited our recent Trends in Genetics review (1997b).  I was astonished when a subsequent speaker, Ed Trifonov, suggested that a plausible metaphor for the behavior of simple sequence repeats might be that of "tuning knobs."  When I approached him after his talk with a reprint of our "Evolutionary Tuning Knobs" essay (1997c), his reaction was priceless.  We later collaborated on a 2006 essay for Caporale's volume, The Implicit Genome.  Delightfully (for me), my own participation at this meeting was reported by Elizabeth Pennisi in a "News Focus" article for the journal Science:  "How the Genome Readies Itself for Evolution" ( Science, 21 Aug 1998, Vol 281: 1131-1134, DOI: 10.1126/science.281.5380.1131).

(Incidently, my flight from Memphis to New York to attend this meeting had memorably passed through a spectacular "cathedral" of clouds, as our airliner wove closely among columns of thunderheads that stretched for miles into the distance.  In hindsight this was a remarkable omen for subsequent events in my career.)
 

(1999a)  King, D.G., and M. Soller  Variation and fidelity:  The evolution of simple sequence repeats as functional elements in adjustable genes.  In:  S.P. Wasser, ed., Evolutionary Theory and Processes: Modern Perspectives, pp. 65-82.  Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, The Netherlands.  [Abstract]   [email for PDF: dgking@siu.edu]

This essay, published in a festschrift dedicated to Eviatar Nevo, continued our campaign to publicize the evolutionary implications of simple sequence repeats.
 

(1998)  King, D.G.  Cells "R" Us.  Brain Awareness Week at SIUC.

This entry is the text of a public lecture offered during a program of public educational activities sponsored by the Society for Neuroscience, and the Dana Foundation.  My goal in this presentation was to acquaint attendees with the basic idea that nerve cells, as the working parts of the brain, are intimate participants in sensation and movement.
 

(1997d)  King, D.G.  Is there a role for comparative genetics in neuroethology?  International Society for Neuroethology Newsletter, July 1997.

This newsletter article was effectively a preview of subsequent 2006c and 2013a articles in the same society newsletter.
 

(1997c)  King, D.G., M. Soller, and Y. Kashi  Evolutionary tuning knobsEndeavour 21: 36-40.  [publisher pdf]   [email for PDF: dgking@siu.edu]

This essay properly introduced "tuning knobs" as a metaphor for the functionality of repetitive DNA (first mentioned in abstract two years earlier, in 1995).  This same metaphor had been independently suggested by Ed Trifonov, at a 1998 meeting of the New York Academy of Sciences ("Molecular strategies in biological evolution," organized by Lynn Helena Caporale).  Trifonov's surprised response when I showed him a reprint of this article led eventually to our collaboration on an essay published in 2006a, "Tuning knobs in the genome: Evolution of Simple Sequence Repeats by Indirect Selection."  A few years later he invited me to participate in a conference at Gregor Mendel's abbey in Brno.

I initially encountered the metaphor of "twiddling a knob on a concept" in an essay by Douglas Hofstadter, "Variations on a theme as the crux of creativity," in Scientific American, October 1982 (reprinted in Metamagical Themas: Questing for the Essence of Mind and Pattern, Basic Books 1985).  Variations on a theme are commonplace in biology; at that time I had begun to wonder, How might the genome implement the idea of twiddling a knob on a concept?  I learned of a plausible answer in a 1983 essay, "Updating the theory of mutation," by Drake, Glickman, & Ripley (American Scientist 71:621-630) which described the peculiar mutational characteristics of repetitive DNA.  But it took another decade for evidence to appear that DNA could actually have such a function, leading first to my 1994 letter to Science and eventually to this "Evolutionary Tuning Knobs" essay.
 

Chezi and me
(with Gregor Mendel)

(1997b)  Y. Kashi, D.G. King, and M. Soller  Simple sequence repeats as a source of quantitative genetic variationTrends in Genetics 13: 74-78.  [Abstract]  [email for PDF: dgking@siu.edu]

This review, my first collaborative publication with Morris Soller (Moshe) and his former student Yechezkel Kashi (Chezi), has become one of our most-cited publications (exceeded only by its sequel, 2006b).  It helped establish the idea that simple sequence repeats could be significant sources of genetic variation. 

This collaboration had been prompted by my 1994 letter to Science.  After noticing that letter, Moshe reached out to me from the Hebrew University in Jerusalem, proposing that we work together to publicize our shared idea.  (Soller, coincidently, was an old friend of Ed Simon, my undergraduate genetics professor at Purdue.)  To facilitate our long-distance correspondence, Moshe initiated me into the use of email.  I finally visited Moshe in person in 2007, at his family home in Chicago.  I met Chezi three years later, at a conference (2010b) held at Gregor Mendel's abby in Brno, Czech Republic.
 

(1997a)  King, D.G.  Behavioral adjustments [letter to the editor], American Scientist 85: 300-301.

This letter addressed an essay, "The evolution of arthropod nervous systems," which had been published in the journal's previous issue (Osirio, Bacon & Whitington, Amer. Sci., 85:244-253, May/June 1997).  My letter posed a simple question:  How can a limited genome support extraordinarily facile evolution of instinctive behavior?  It then proposed that the answer might involve repetitive DNA.  Alongside this letter, the journal published a response by the authors of the original essay, expressing strongly-worded skepticism.  The basis for their skeptism was fully addressed in several of our subsequent publications (e.g., 2008a), culminating in my 2013a essay for the International Society for Neuroethology.  (For a related, extended discussion, see my 2001 online-only essay, "Evolution of neural circuits.")
 

(1996)  Krajewski, C. and D.G. King,  Molecular divergence and phylogeny:  Rates and patterns of cytochrome b evolution in cranes.  Molecular Biology and Evolution 13: 21-30.   [Abstract]

Carey Krajewski is a friend and colleague at SIUC Zoology.  For this and a subsequent 1999c paper also coauthored with Krajewski, my only contribution was the coding of conceptually-simple computer programs which facilitated the comparison of DNA sequences.
 

(1995)  King, D.G.Triplet-repeat DNA: Unstable regulatory genes could be evolutionary "tuning knobs."  (abstract only)

This abstract for a Neuroscience meeting represents my first published mention of the "tuning knobs" metaphor, published more substantially two years later (1997c).
 

(1994)  King, D.G.  Triple repeat DNA as a highly mutable regulatory mechanism.  Science 263: 595-596.  [publisher pdf]   [email for PDF: dgking@siu.edu]

This letter-to-the-editor (appearing in print just in time for my 46th birthday!) was prompted by a news article in Science ("The Puzzle of the Triple Repeats," 4 June 1993), which had reported a peculiar quantitative relationship between severity of symptoms in Huntington's disease and number of CAG repeats in the causative gene.  This relationship corresponded remarkably well with a speculation I had conceived a decade earlier, while developing the concept of "metaptation."

By staking my claim to the hypothesis that repetitive DNA might confer upon genes the property of evolutionary adjustability, this letter officially marked the beginning of my re-invention as a theoretical evolutionary geneticist.  I was over the moon when, one week after this letter appeared, Science published a paper by Gerber et al. which powerfully confirmed the plausibility of my hypothesis.

Publication of this letter led directly to collaboration with Yechezkel Kashi and Morris Soller, of the Hebrew University in Jerusalem, who had independently developed similar ideas.  Together we have coauthored several papers, beginning with Trends in Genetics (1997b) and Endeavour (1997c). 
 

(1991)  King, D.G.  The origin of an organ:  Phylogenetic analysis of evolutionary innovation in the digestive tract of flies (Insecta: Diptera).  Evolution 45:568-588.  [email for PDF: dgking@siu.edu]

Research leading to this report was probably the most fun of any in my career.  It involved collecting and studying many diverse fly species; it provided intense engagement with evolutionary thinking and phylogenetic reasoning; and it resulted in an elegant story of how a fairly complex organ had evolved.  I became inordinately pleased with myself -- a neuroscientist by training -- for successfully publishing in a journal devoted to evolutionary biology.  (I had not yet begun my full transformation into an evolutionary geneticist.)  Although this report has not been cited nearly as often as several subsequent papers on molecular evolution (e.g., 1997b, 1997c, 2006b), it remains one of my favorite publications.
 

(1989)  King, D.G.  Phylogenetic diversity of cellular organization in the cardia of muscoid flies (Diptera: Schizophora).  Journal of Morphology 202: 435-455.  [email for PDF: dgking@siu.edu]

This publication set the stage for my 1991 report on the evolutionary origin of the dipteran cardia.  It was based on a collection of various fly species initially intended for exploration of the phylogenetic origin of giant axons found in Drosophila, a study eventually published in the ISN newsletter in 2013a.
 

(1988b)  King, D.G.  On the evolution of specialized neuronal form: Phylogenetic variation in cervical giant axons of flies (Diptera).  Proc. XVIII Int. Congress of Entomology, p. 70 (abstract only).

Travel to this Congress of Entomology meeting was especially memorable:  My wife and I drove from southern Illinois to the meeting site in Vancouver, B.C., touring the country for five weeks with our son and daughter, ages 6 and 9.

Several years earlier (in 1980), I had described a giant-axon pathway in Drosophila.  Shortly afterwards I had established that a homologous giant-axon pathway, although missing in tsetse flies (1983a), could nevertheless be found in other muscoid flies (1983b).  In an effort to establish when this giant-axon pathway had first evolved, I set out to map its phylogenetic distribution by examining as many different flies as I could easily acquire.  This provided me with a welcome excuse for outdoor fieldwork, collecting specimens around the delightful environs of southern Illinois.  Eventually, by the time of this meeting, I had recorded axon size distributions in sixty-five species representing thirty-six families. 

Although observations presented at this Vancouver meeting did not suggest a simple story about the evolutionary origin of Drosophila giant fibers, they were nonetheless quite intriguing:  Axon size distributions among various fly species were unexpectedly diverse, even within a single family.  A few images of this diversity were eventually published 25 years later.  And the same collection of specimens did serve admirably for my analysis of the evolutionary origin of the dipteran cardia (in 1989 and 1991).
 

(1988a)  King, D.G.  Cellular organization and peritrophic membrane formation in the cardia (proventriculus) of Drosophila melanogaster.  Journal of Morphology 196: 253-282.  [Abstract] [email for PDF: dgking@siu.edu]

This study was prompted by accidental observation of abnormal cardia development in some behavioral mutants of Drosophila, whereupon I realized that this organ had never received a detailed histological description.  This was my first substantial work that had not been grounded on work in the laboratories of my doctoral and post-doctoral advisors.  It led directly to my 1989 and 1991 publications (above) on comparative anatomy of the Dipteran cardia. 

I might never have paid attention to the fly cardia, but for its location atop the thoracic nervous system where I could not help but notice it while studying nerve fibers.
 

(1985)  King, D.G.    Metaptation: A descriptive category for evolutionarily versatile patterns of genetic and ontogenetic organization.  Abstract only, in Evolutionary Theory 7:222.  [Abstract]  [unpublished ms.]

Here I must acknowledge a special debt to Steven Jay Gould.  Gould's monthly essays in Natural History magazine inspired me to wonder about evolvability.  I shared some initial thoughts in a "fan letter" to Gould.  Without the encouragement he offered in his generous reply, I probably would have lacked confidence to continue writing in a domain of biology where I had no formal training. 

This abstract marked the beginning of a major shift in my career orientation, from neuroscience to evolutionary genetics, culminating in a pre-retirement 2012 Annals NYAS paper and a post-retirement 2024 Journal of Physiology paper, both on mutation protocols. 

The title neologism here is modelled after Gould & Vrba's coinage of "exaptation."  Twenty-five years later, at a 2010b meeting in the Czech Republic, I presented a "new, improved" definition for the word. 

The word metaptation has not yet entered mainstream usage, although it has been adopted by at least one researcher working with the evolvability of computer programs (Moshe Looks 2007, Program evolution for general intelligence, full text) and is elaborated in William Calvin's book The River that Flows Uphill (1986, 2010).  I took advantage of my post-retirement 2024 Journal of Physiology article to slip the word "metaptation" into mainstream biology literature.
Making variations on a theme is really the crux of creativity.
Douglas Hofstadter 1982
This 1985 abstract was originally written to accompany a lengthy essay that had been written in 1983 and submitted to a journal where it languished for two years before being rejected.  (Meanwhile I had moved on with histological study of fly anatomy.)  The original Metaptation manuscript included my first recognition of the plausibility of "evolutionary tuning knobs," an idea that was inspired by a 1982 essay by Douglas Hofstadter but not published properly until 1997
 

(1984)  Wyman, R.J., J.B. Thomas, L. Salkoff, and D.G. King  The escape response of Drosophila melanogaster.  In:  Neural Mechanisms of Startle Behavior (R. Eaton, Ed.), Plenum Press, New York.

My inclusion as an author of this review, based on my participation as a postdoc in Robert Wyman's lab at Yale (1976-1977, with publication in (1980), was strictly honorary. 
 

(1983e)  King, D.G.  Review of Genetic Neurobiology, by Hall, Greenspan, & Harris; in Quarterly Review of Biology, 58:286.

When I wrote this very brief book review (less than 1/2 page), genetics was still a fairly new tool for studying nervous systems.  I have since come to believe that additional value can be obtained by using some of the more remarkable features of nervous systems as inspiration for re-thinking certain aspects of genetics (cf, What can giant axons tell us about genetics?, above).
 

(1983d)  King, D.G. and M.A. Tanouye  Anatomy of motor axons to direct flight muscles in Drosophila.  Journal of Experimental Biology 105: 231-239. [email for PDF: dgking@siu.edu]

While working as a postdoc in Robert Wyman's lab at Yale, I had been delighted to discover that individual motor axons in a small animal the size of Drosophila could be reliably traced through serial thin sections all the way from their neuromuscular junctions into the thoracic central nervous system.  This report of my histological observations was submitted jointly with Mark Tanouye, along with a companion paper (1983c, below) reporting Tanouye's recordings of electrical activity from these muscles. 
 

(1983c)  Tanouye, M.A. and D.G. King  Giant fiber activation of direct flight muscles in Drosophila.  Journal of Experimental Biology 105: 241-251. [email for PDF: dgking@siu.edu]

Two companion papers were submitted jointly with Mark Tanouye, a friend from our time together in Robert Wyman's lab at Yale.  This one, with Tanouye as principal author, reported physiological recording from Drosophila flight muscles.  My contribution (1983d, above) was histological identification of sites where dye had been injected during recording.
 

(1983b)  King, D.G. and K.L. Valentino  On neuronal homology:  A comparison of similar axons in Musca, Sarcophaga and Drosophila (Diptera:  Schizophora).  Journal of Comparative Neurology 219: 1-9.   [Abstract]

Establishing homology at the level of individual cells is fraught with both technical and conceptual difficulties.  This article discusses these challenges while reporting putative homology for a set of axons individually identified in three fly species from three different taxonomic families.  This was accomplished primarily by demonstrating that their similarity extended to the level of synaptic location.  Unpublished continuation of this work has demonstrated the same configuration of synapsing axons in several additional fly species.

At the time this work was begun, Karen Valentino was an undergraduate student in Robert Wyman's lab at Yale.  While working with Wyman as a postdoc, I offered her technical guidance as she prepared images of one of the specimens which eventually appeared in this report.
 

(1983a)  King, D.G.  Evolutionary loss of a neural pathway from the nervous system of a fly (Glossina morsitans, Diptera).  Journal of Morphology 175: 27-32.  [Abstract]

Together with my paper "On neuronal homology..." (1983, above), this publication represented my initial foray into comparative study and evolutionary thinking.

My interest in an evolutionary perspective had been kindled by Steven Jay Gould's monthly essays in Natural History magazine, under the heading "This view of life."
 

(1982b)  King, D.G., N. Kammlade, and J. Murphy  A simple device to help reembed plastic sections.  Stain Technology 57: 307-311.  [Abstract]

Much of my histological study of flies involved relatively thick (2-5μm) serial sections of plastic-embedded specimens.  With some help from the gadget described in this report, selected sections could then be lifted off their glass slides and re-sectioned for electron microscopy.  (Ideally, as many as twenty sections for electron microscopy might be cut parallel to the face of one 2μm thick section.)
 

(1980)  King, D.G. and R.J. Wyman  Anatomy of the giant fiber pathway in Drosophila:  I. Three thoracic components of the pathway.  Journal of Neurocytology 9: 753-770.  [Abstract]

This publication resulted from work completed during my post-doctoral experience (1976-1977) with Robert Wyman at Yale, where my attention was redirected from examining identified nerve cells in lobster to investigating the unique giant axons in Drosophila.  The Roman numeral in the title betrays my optimistic but mistaken expectation that an additional publication would soon follow.  I subsequently sought giant axons in numerous other fly species, collecting images which were eventually published in 2013a in an essay entitled "What can giant axons tell us about genetics and evolution?"
 

(1978)  Wyman, R.J. and D.G. King, The dispersion of imaginal disc progenitor cells in the Drosophila blastoderm.  Genetic Research 31: 273-286.  [Abstract]

My postdoctoral mentor, Robert Wyman, was the principal author for this paper.  My contribution -- a simple mathematical analysis of a simplistic model of the formation of genetic mosaics in Drosophila -- included a hand-inked diagram (at right) which inadvertently resembled the "death star" in the 1977 movie Star Wars.
 

(1976c)  King, D.G.  Organization of crustacean neuropil:  II. Distribution of synaptic contacts on identified motor neurons in lobster stomatogastric ganglion.  Journal of Neurocytology 5: 239-266.  [Abstract] [email for PDF: dgking@siu.edu]

This paper, together with its companion paper (1976b, below), comprised my doctoral dissertation in neuroscience at UCSD.  Remarkably, this paper is still being cited more than 40 years after publication. 

This report is based on a single specimen whose cells had been individually identified electrophysiologically by my dissertation advisor, Allen Selverston.  Serial-sectioning this specimen for electron microscopy enabled reconstruction of the shape of each cell's principal branches, including localization of specific synapses.  Cutting, staining, and imaging approximately 5000 sections through this small piece of tissue (less than a millimeter in length), together with analyzing the resulting micrographs, was by far the most strenuous single piece of lab-work in my career.

The phrase "identified motor neurons" in this paper's title refers to uniquely identifiable nerve cells.  This concept -- still fairly new in the 1970s and remaining relatively unfamiliar even into the 21st century -- became a guiding theme in my subsequent research.  Without realizing it at the time, I was both lucky and privileged to be a student at an institution where "one of the major revolutions in neuroscience" was underway!
 

(1976b)  King, D.G. Organization of crustacean neuropil:  I. Patterns of synaptic connections in lobster stomatogastric ganglion.  Journal of Neurocytology 5: 207-237.  [Abstract]  [email for full-text PDF: dgking@siu.edu]

This paper, together with its companion paper (1976c, above), comprised my doctoral dissertation in neuroscience at the University of California San Diego, completed with extensive support through the laboratory of Allen Selverston.
 

(1976a)  Selverston, A., D.R. Russell, J.P. Miller, and D.G. King,  The stomatogastric nervous system:  Structure and function of a small neural network.  Progress in Neurobiology 7: 215-290.

For several decades, a number of laboratories around the world have been intensively investigating the crustacean stomatogastric nervous system -- a model system for analyzing the mechanisms of motor pattern generation.  I completed my own graduate study at one center for such study, Allen Selverston's lab at UC San Diego.  This publication is a review of work up to 1975.  [More recent reviews appeared in 1987, and 2016.]  My principal contribution (in addition to assisting with overall editing) was a single-chapter preview of my doctoral dissertation, published separately and in greater detail in the two 1976 publications listed immediately above.  (Coauthors Russell and Miller were also graduate students in Selverston's lab.) 
 

until 1980    |   1981-1990    |   1991-2000    |   2001-2010    |   since 2010    |  Epilogue

Return to King homepage


David King portrait Comments and questions: dgking@siu.edu

SIUC / Zoology / David King
https://dgkinglab.siu.edu/PUBLIST.HTM
Last updated:  18 January 2024 / dgk